rss
Postgrad Med J 83:518-524 doi:10.1136/pgmj.2006.053066
  • Review

Personal development plans and self-directed learning for healthcare professionals: are they evidence based?

  1. Stephen F Jennings
  1. Correspondence to:
 Dr Stephen F Jennings
 Faculty of Health, Staffordshire University, Blackheath Lane, Stafford, ST18 0AD, UK; sfjmedical{at}btinternet.com
  • Received 1 September 2006
  • Accepted 29 November 2006

Abstract

The UK chief medical officer’s recommendations for the re-licensing and performance management of doctors will mean a move from a formative towards a summative role for appraisal and its adjunct, the personal development plan. Where does this leave medical educators trying to promote reflective learning? It is taken for granted that self-directed learning is the sine qua non of all adult learning. But is it? This review re-evaluates self-directed learning and its corollary, the personal development plan, in the light of the chief medical officer’s report, seeking the evidence behind today’s accepted educational practice. It discovers a reality which challenges assumptions long enshrined in medical education.

Until now, UK doctors have had no obligation to demonstrate their continuing fitness to practise. In calling for National Health Service developmental appraisal to measure performance and become the basis for re-licensure,1 the chief medical officer (CMO) has effectively moved the personal development plan (PDP) into the minimum standards arena and called into question its use as a formative development tool. This literature review suggests he may be right.

Grant and Stanton’s influential 1999 review into the effectiveness of continuing professional development2 called for a focus on the process, based on individual needs. This resulted in the reforms of appraisal and the PDP, now contractual requirements for most UK doctors, who will often be familiar with them through their training schemes. But the PDP diverts the focus from process to content. It can be argued that by restricting study to the items on the plan, it promotes prescriptive over experiential learning. Tutors have subsequently attempted to increase health professionals’ control of their learning by making it an object of reflection. This may work for some, but not all. Candy3 pointed out in 1991: “There may be limited transfer of self-directed competence from one context to the next and the pursuit of generalised strategies is probably ill advised and foredoomed.” Is self-directed learning so important? Is there any evidence that PDPs are effective?

WHAT IS SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING?

All adults learn; 80–100% of adults engage in some form of learning activity throughout their lives,3 but only 20% of these do so in a formal educational setting.4 Knowles defined self-directed learning5 as a process in which individuals take the initiative in diagnosing their learning needs, designing learning experiences, locating resources and evaluating their learning. From fashioning a coracle, to the Socratic method of thinking for oneself, it implies a high order cognitive activity, and one which distinguishes the adult learner from the child and adolescent. But in enunciating the assumptions of andragogy6 (“the art and science of helping adults learn”) Knowles inadvertently heralded a new philosophy, setting Freire’s Pedagogy of the oppressed7 in a logical theoretical framework; adult educators searching for a professional identity seized upon his unproven assumptions as axioms8,9 which took self-directed learning as the sine qua non of all adult learning. But is it?

WHAT SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING IS NOT

Self-directed learning is not a philosophy, nor is it a set of techniques to be applied by an institution wanting to teach a self-directed programme. It is an internalised process related to motivation and self-identity,8 something that happens within a person, not something that is done to them. Where is the evidence for this?

Tough10 confirmed that when adults decide to learn they first invest time and energy in checking the potential benefits; as Knowles put it, “Learners need to know why they need to know”. But Tough also found that those adults devalue their work if not validated by some external authority. Hence the rational for Brookfield’s principle of effective facilitation8: the self-directed-learner must be supported and reassured. Yet it is he who points out that this has now become unchallenged academic orthodoxy.8

WHY ARE SOME LEARNERS MORE SELF-DIRECTED THAN OTHERS?

Since there are many alternative models (table 1),11–20 why must it be necessary or even desirable for adults to be self-directed in order to learn? Brookfield points out that self-directed learning alone has less successful outcomes than a mix of self-directed learning and group learning.21 If all adults are self-directed, why are some more self-directed than others?22 What factors determine how self-directed an individual is?

Table 1

 Learner-centred models of learning

In his own literature review, Candy3 found that a number of paired traits had been associated with the ideal of self-directedness—for example, logical/analytical, curious/open, reflective/self aware—suggesting that it was not a feature of every adult learner. Andragogy would have teachers attempt to develop these traits in learners, to make them more self-directed. Teachers can encourage curiosity, but can they make someone more open-minded, more logical? The work of Jung23 and Myers24 suggested this might depend more on behavioural types.

DOES SELF-DIRECTION DEPEND ON LEARNING STYLE?

In Kolb’s model of experiential learning, learners choose how to acquire information (either concretely through the senses or abstractly by analysing) and process it (reflectively by watching or actively by doing); hence the learning style inventory25 and the learning cycle.26 To Honey and Mumford27 Kolb’s assimilators became reflector-theorists; accommodators became activist-pragmatists, etc (fig 1).

Figure 1

 Kolb’s experiential learning styles are in black, Honey and Mumford’s in red. Adapted from Robinson.34

To Riding and Cheema28 and Rayner29 the key was not learning style but cognitive style. This develops between 6 and 9 years old, not in adolescence.30 Curry showed how these may be related, with cognitive style more deeply embedded than learning style.31

Baker et al,32 Allinson et al33 and Robinson34 linked a propensity to take risks with the activist/accommodator-converger styles. Reflector/assimilator-divergers have some preference for lectures and non-group activities; they deliberately exclude themselves from risk. Could it be that they are therefore more self-directed? In 1984 Pratt35 enunciated what many had already assumed, that self-directed learning is associated with autonomy—that is, with Witkin’s (1949) “field-independent” thinkers.36 These field-independents have close connections with the reflector/assimilator-diverger learning style. It was therefore a logical deduction, and one that fitted nicely with the aspirations of adult educators, that self-directed learners would tend to be reflectors, and therefore in order to support learners in becoming more self-directed, they should be encouraged to adopt a more reflective style. In spite of its undeniable logic, common sense and convenience, this deduction was spurious.8 Brookfield had already demonstrated the reverse.37

In work later corroborated by Thiel,38 Brookfield had looked at real, successful self-directed adult learners and found they were more likely to be field-dependent, heuristic, activist-accommodators. The key was that these were extrinsically motivated and gregarious, willing to network in order to problem solve. The autonomous field-independent/reflectors kept themselves to themselves and sought less help. Although field-dependent/activist-pragmatists looked to others to mediate their learning, they possessed the single most important “measure of self-direction”39,40—their ability to act on their critical reflection. The reflectors merely reflected. As Eva discussed in an editorial in 2005, the key determinants of self-direction might not be learning style, nor indeed personality type, but the meta-cognitive processes that determine both.41

IS CURRENT PRACTICE EVIDENCE BASED?

Why then, 20 years after Brookfield pointed all this out, are health professionals being forced to use PDPs to “become more reflective” when this could demotivate the most self-directed? True, Brookfield says they need support—just not in this way. Could it be that medical educators have reluctantly accepted this practice, because the alternatives in maintaining medical competence are too draconian? Or is the heuristic, trial and error approach, for millennia the mainstay of scientific advance, hard to justify in 21st century medico-legal terms?

The canon of evidence on self-directed learning in healthcare is legion, but much of it is not directly comparable since “experts” mean different things by self-directed learning. The basic model sees control transferred from teacher to learner with growing independence over time (fig 2).

Figure 2

 A simple model of self-direction. Adapted from Candy.3

But this is too simplistic. Candy3 reminds us that the term self-direction as referred to in the literature means four different things (table 2). The first two are concerned with self-direction as method or process: (1) the degree of control a learner has over the mode of instruction; (2) autodidaxy (“teach-yourself”, or the independent pursuit of learning). The next two are concerned with self-direction as outcome or goal: (3) self-management of learning; (4) self-determination of destiny.

Table 2

 Four domains of “self-direction”

Each domain can be represented as the change in control over time (for example, the instructional domain shown in fig 3).

Figure 3

 Candy’s learner-control continuum: instructional domain. Adapted from Candy3 and Millar.42

SIX PROBLEMS

Dittman43 points out that some educators (for example, Marbeau44) wrongly assumed that simply “doing self-directed learning” at an instructional or autodidactic level leads to autonomy (fig 4), and that these four different but overlapping domains can be superimposed chronologically on the simplistic model (for example, Davis and Thompson45).

Figure 4

 Domains of self direction: idealised version. From an idea in Candy.3

In reality these occur randomly (fig 5), as different people operate in different domains at different times in their lives, as a result of their upbringing, previous learning experiences and the “organising circumstances” over which they have little control.46 So they will choose to be more or less self-directed depending upon their constructions and their confidence in a particular subject at a particular time.47 In other words, self-directed learning is context specific.48

Figure 5

 Domains of self direction: a realistic example. From an idea in Candy.3

Second, given that self-directed learning is context specific, it is learners’ experiences that are important in any research, not their test results, since those may vary with the context. Qualitative studies are therefore most suitable for all but the instructional domain of this relativist topic.49 Yet they are swamped by quantitative ones.50,51 Meta-analyses only compound the error.

Third, research conclusions from studies in one domain alone are applied erroneously as evidence across all domains—for example, randomised controlled studies comparing traditional teaching approaches with specific techniques that allow the learner a small degree of choice, are valid only in the instructional domain, yet purport to demonstrate the effectiveness of self-directed learning globally or across disciplines.3

Fourth, it is disingenuous to restrict learner-control to a limited choice—for example, between lectures or a CD-ROM. Learning is holistic. As Boucouvalas and Pearse state: “When learners set the outcomes, that is self-directed learning. When teachers set the outcomes, that is benevolent pedagogy.”52 Which of these models best fits undergraduate medicine in the UK in 2007? It may be appropriate to teach medicine in this way because medical students are not capable of assessing their learning needs,53 but why call it self-directed learning?

Fifth, there is overwhelming evidence that doctors also are incapable of assessing their learning needs accurately.45,54–57 “If I am authentically learning I will not be able to predict where I go with accuracy. If I could predict with accuracy…I already know what I am purporting to learn…I have already arrived at where I have decided to go. The goal specified…is not the true goal. The true goal, as Sartre has indicated, is what we desire at the end…not at the beginning” (Burstow58). Or as Keil put it: “How can I know what I don’t know when I don’t know what I don’t know?”59 Wherefore the PDP now?

Finally, there is no evidence that self-directed as opposed to teacher-directed learning improves learning outcomes.60–67

WHY SELF-DIRECTED LEARNING?

Self-directed learning has been shown to be associated with increased curiosity, critical thinking, quality of understanding, retention and recall, better decision making, achievement satisfaction, motivation, competence and confidence.65,67,68 These are all important qualities in doctors, not measurable by quantitative studies, and reason enough to support self-direction. The value in doing so is not to optimise the learning outcomes in terms of scores on a multiple choice questionnaire but, by encouraging participation, to reduce the numbers of demotivated doctors who drop out of learning.69,70 Performing well in any task requires cognitive and meta-cognitive functioning: knowledge of the task, and knowledge of one’s own motives, resources and constraints in context, in order to plan strategically. As Biggs states, the task of learning is no different.71 Therefore self-directed learning, which exemplifies this par excellence, as well as being the most natural form of learning, is also associated with high quality learning.72

In the setting of UK general medical practice, relative professional isolation means that self-direction is a fact of life. Therefore the onus is on facilitators to help general practitioners (GPs) learn effectively that which is relevant. Continuing medical education based on personal preference is now perceived to be financially untenable in a system of rationed healthcare,73,74 and has been superseded by a programme of appraised personal development planning, optimistically based on assessment of personal learning and service needs. The formative appraisal part of the process has been largely welcomed and valued by the profession.75 Acceptance and usefulness of the PDP is less clear. Its aims were supposedly to increase autonomous reflective practice (which is perceived as “safer for patients” than heuristic learning) by attempting to make GPs more self-directed, and thereby improve health outcomes. But the link with revalidation has only confirmed the political agenda in imposing “self-direction” on an already quite autonomous group of professionals76 and there is no theory or existing evidence to support any of it, although the research methodology to date has been flawed, debunked by Brookfield and Candy years ago.

WHY PERSONAL DEVELOPMENT PLANS?

The PDP was already out of date when introduced in the UK, based on an entirely fallacious understanding of self-directed learning, and discredited by Frewin in 1976.77 The plan stemmed from Flanagan’s 1970 assertion78 that “a self-directed learner should have a reasonable degree of skill and decision making in planning, which should include…the ability to analyse and define problems and prepare a sequential plan using a clear statement of desired outcomes and working back to obtain a definite schedule and a set of procedures for determining the required progress at each point.” Indeed she should. But to paraphrase Candy, what if she is not a reflector with mechanical, linear and analytical cognitive processes? What use is this plan to her? It sits in a draw, giving the impression that the mandate to ensure the doctor’s competence has been filled,79 but is in reality redundant. Evans80 suggested PDPs were well received by 80% of GPs, influencing their development and quality of patient care, but the study pre-dated UK national appraisal and those GPs chosen had elected to use a PDP, suggesting that this appealed to their learning preferences. Subsequent studies which reduced sample bias are more circumspect81; 50% of GPs regard PDPs as hoops to be jumped.82 Their mechanistic production devalues the self-directed element of formative appraisal.83,84

The concept of the self-directed learner is more complex than fitting a simple reflective model. People’s propensity to control their own learning is not necessarily determined by increasing their reflective abilities, but by meta-cognitive processes which depend upon personality type, learning style preference, cognitive style, past experiences, situation pertaining, subject studied, acquired competencies, or all or none of these. A PDP may help or hinder.85 For doctors who proceed heuristically, lapping the learning cycle many times a day, planning learning a year in advance is demotivating as well as pointless, due to the sense of inadequacy engendered. But these may well be self-directed learners. They are not unsafe practitioners. Schön’s “reflection-in-action” (table 1) is not only more relevant for them, but may be more important than “reflection-on-action” generally, for it occurs at the coal face of decision making. We may know what we should do in a situation, but it is what we actually do that affects patients. Bandura’s judgements in self-efficacy (table 1) are influenced by heuristics.86 Appraisal and reflective practice, as currently advocated across all health professions, emphasise reflection-on-action and ignore reflection-in-action, and we know that doctors are bad at identifying their own weaknesses when they do reflect-on-action.59,87,88 They are not unique; self-belief is a selective advantage89 and this casts doubt on the relevance of self-assessment. Does it really address unconscious incompetence? Although reflection-in-action can be unconscious it appears to involve a stepping up of cognitive resources90 and is modifiable.91 Patients’ unmet needs (PUNS) and doctors’ educational needs (DENS) attempt to crystallise the immediacy of this reflection-in-action. Reflection-on-action is still important yet the paradox of the PDP is that it is potentially a useful learning tool for those to whom it holds least appeal but who need a structured approach-wholist activist/pragmatists, who tend to include on it tasks they have already completed; it is probably of no use to those analyst/theorist-reflectors who have their own more detailed portfolio anyway, but quite enjoy filling in the PDP. At worst, it is counterproductive for the enthusiastic obsessive who fails to complete an unrealistic self-delegated agenda.

If it is to remain, the PDP will need to be short term, tailored to the individual and incorporating strategic challenges, not just in order to appeal to the activist learner but because, as Björk has shown,92 “desirable difficulties” increase the accuracy of self-assessment, which nicely fits the cognitive research93 as well as Freire’s praxis.94 The beauty of self-directed learning is that even without a PDP, it overrides all these complex issues and opts, by definition, for the most appropriate route to personal learning, although still requiring support. But without that PDP, how can the facilitator determine what has or will be learned? Herein lies the rub: the PDP probably has more use to educators and assessors, than learners.

The evidence base on self-directed learning

  • There is no evidence that self-directed learning leads to autonomy

  • Research conclusions are being applied erroneously

  • Learners’ experiences are key, not their results

  • Restricting learner-control has no part in self-directed learning

  • Doctors are incapable of accurately assessing their learning needs

  • There is no evidence that self-directed learning improves outcomes But…

  • Self-directed learning is high quality learning

ALTERNATIVES?

Grant has subsequently pointed out that needs assessment alone can stifle creative learning and does not improve outcomes unless in the appropriate context.95 She also reasserts the findings of Davis96 and Bandara and Calvert83 that doctors already have their own, if flawed, ways of assessing their needs and asks if these should be the starting point of new systems for formative professional improvement.

The focus for the immediate future needs to be on the individual, not the process or content. As Asadoorian and Batty state,97 we need facilitation based on the needs of the individual, and tailored to individual preference, using personalised tools ascertained from each individual appraisal. Lyons has called for a system that considers individuals’ learning and cognitive styles.98 We need to inculcate “personalised, surgery-based climates of enquiry”. We need qualitative research into the value individuals derive from the process, into individual perceptions of the effect of PDPs on reflective practice, and into relevant individual health outcomes99; population benefits may come later. We will need to find an alternative to PDPs at least for formative purposes—they may still have a role in performance appraisal. That alternative must allow doctors to inform their future learning by whatever method they choose, as long as the outcome is patient centred. But qualitative research should precede hypotheses, so we will have to wait until we know the results, before we can predict what form the PDP replacement should take. It should be remembered though that doctors still have a responsibility to manage and resource their own development,84 and as we know from Brookfield, will still require some form of ongoing support with appraisal.

Personal developments plans (PDPs)

  • PDPs are founded on a misconception

  • The practice of promoting reflective learning via PDPs is not evidence based

  • The PDP is probably more useful to the facilitator than the learner

  • A PDP is not essential for successful self-directed learning

Key references

Further reading

To quote Newman and Peile: “Any move towards common standards in education must not obscure the need for medical educators to remain flexible and agile in responding to different learners’ needs. Educators need to improve their skills in facilitation, judicious application of educational theory and mentoring.”100

CONCLUSION

The aims of andragogy and self-directed learning were honourable. They should be pursued in a reasoned, informed, evidence-based fashion in continuing medical education, acknowledging complexity and avoiding unsubstantiated claims, academic orthodoxy and political expedience. Let the right research questions be asked and the right research methods be used. Let what has already been written be read again carefully, to distinguish fact from fiction. Let there be a sound basis for the support of self-directed health professionals.

Footnotes

  • Conflict of interest: none stated

REFERENCES


Free sample
This recent issue is free to all users to allow everyone the opportunity to see the full scope and typical content of PMJ.
View free sample issue >>

Don't forget to sign up for content alerts so you keep up to date with all the articles as they are published.